New research shows the cognitive differences between men and women are environmental rather than genetic

hunterMen and women think differently. Ask my wife and she’ll explain this in some detail. But why we do think differently is another mystery.

Going back in time, one possible theory is that when we were hunter/gatherers different skills were required. The men spent time away from camp, tracking animals and fighting off intruders. Women needed social skills to bring up children.

Yet there are bound to be many other factors at work for this variation to survive into modern times.

The latest research suggests that living standards and access to education probably bear more responsibility for cognitive disparity between men and women than genes, nursery colours or the ability to catch a ball.

Previous studies have shown that male and female brains are wired differently.

Last year Ragini Verma of the University of Pennsylvania used sophisticated imaging techniques to show variations between men and women in dominant connections in the cerebrum, the part of the brain that does the thinking.

Dr Verma speculated this could help explain why women tend to have better memories, social adeptness and an improved ability to multitask.

Daniela Weber and her colleagues at the International Institute for Applied Systems Analysis in Vienna suggest why such changes come about and how the differences can change.

The group’s analysis, reported in the venerable Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, finds that the cognitive performance of women—much more so than men—benefits from factors such as greater employment opportunities, increased economic prosperity and better health.

oldiesThese findings are based on interviews with 17,000 men and 14,000 women carried out by an initiative called the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe, conducted in 2006-07.

The interviewees, aged 50 to 84 years-old lived in 13 European countries and were subjected to three tests for cognitive performance. They were grouped into three geographic regions: northern Europe, central Europe and southern Europe.

For each country a regional development index (RDI) was adopted, an amalgam of GDP, family size, infant mortality, life expectancy and national education levels.

These criteria were selected to represent a country’s educational and living standards for the birth years of the interviewees, and then compared with participants’ cognitive performance.

That performance was determined using tests for episodic memory (the retention of words in memory), category fluency (naming examples of, say, animals) and numeracy.

In these tests, women were expected to outperform men in episodic memory and men do better in numeracy. Neither sex is thought dominant in category fluency.

Episodic memory matters because it’s linked to emotion. The brain remembers unconnected words by linking them to a memory or imagined situation. It’s the emotion of the memory that supposedly helps the brain remember the word.

Whether women actually are more empathetic than men is debatable, unless you’re my wife that is.

It may be that society has expected such capacity from the principal child-carers for so long that it’s become ingrained. The same goes for numeracy.

Is science dominated by men because they’re better at it or because it was a career choice not widely open to women before the late 20th century?

The new study doesn’t seek to answer these questions.

But it pushes research further towards answers. As expected, in episodic-memory tests women outperformed or were equal to men in every geographic area and across almost all age groups.

The results for numeracy were even starker.

Men outperformed women in all areas and in all age groups.

business-womanThe differences in category fluency were so slight as to be statistically irrelevant. But the really startling discovery was how the results changed over time and by region.

As countries improved in RDI terms, the cognitive performance of the whole population was raised. Northern Europe rated highest across all age groups, followed by central and southern Europe.

Unexpectedly, the study found that the better developed a country, the higher the rate of increase in women’s cognitive abilities. My wife will be pleased.

However, quite why women benefit disproportionately from societal improvements remains a mystery.

Some researchers think that women may be starting from a more disadvantaged level and have a greater potential to catch up as the RDI score improves.

Generally, the cognitive differences in numeracy between men and women are narrowing, more so in northern Europe than in southern Europe.

North European women aged 50-54 years-old are also increasing their lead over men in episodic memory, by remembering almost one more word out of ten.

Whatever the reason, this study indicates that cognitive differences between men and women are not solely inherited.

And it suggests these cognitive differences are learned from the roles a society expects males and females to perform, and that those differences can change as society changes.

So the variation in the cognitive abilities of the two sexes may be less to do gender stereotypes as we may have first thought and much more to do with social development in the 21st Century.

Leave a reply